User:Epottala/sandbox

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Language Acquisition and Prelingual Deafness[edit]

Speech Acquisition[edit]

Deaf children do not acquire speech the same as hearing children because they cannot hear the language spoken around them. In normal language acquisition, auditory comprehension precedes the development of language.[1] Without auditory input, a person with prelingual deafness is forced to acquire speech visually through lip-reading. Acquiring spoken language through lip-reading alone is challenging for the deaf child because it does not always accurately represent speech sounds.[2] The likelihood of a deaf child successfully learning to speak is based on a variety of factors including: ability to discriminate between speech sounds, a higher than average non-verbal IQ, and a higher socioeconomic status.[3] Despite being fitted with hearing aids or provided with oral instruction and speech therapy at a young age, prelingually deaf children are unlikely to ever develop perfect speech and speech-reception skills.[4] Some researchers conclude that deaf children taught exclusively through spoken language appear to pass through the same general stages of language acquisition as their hearing peers but without reaching the same ultimate level of proficiency. [5] The spoken language that may develop for prelingually deaf children will be severely delayed.

Cochlear Implants[edit]

Speech perception can be corrected prior to language acquisition with cochlear implants. After a year and a half experience, researchers found the deaf culture was able to identify words and comprehend movements of others' lips. There is a greater opportunity to hear a sound depending on the location of electrodes compared to the tissue and the number of remaining neurons located in the auditory system.[6] In addition, individual capacities as well as the neural supply to the cochlea play a role in the process of learning with cochlear implantation.

It is recommended for children with prelingual deafness to receive the implantation as soon as possible. Studies continue to show that children with prelingual deafness are able interact in society comfortably when implantation occurs before the age of five.[7] Speech production is a slower procedure in the beginning since creating words requires more effort. Children who had almost two years experience with cochlear implants were able to generate diphthongs and sound out most vowels.[8] They develop skills to understand more information as well as put together letters.

Cochlear implants give deaf individuals the chance to understand auditory messages.[9] Progress was analyzed after several groups of children were given vocabulary and language tests. After three years of practice, the children with the devices did as well as children that had no previous issues with hearing. Specifically, cochlear implants allow children with prelingual deafness to acquire skills similar to children with minimal or no residual hearing.[10]

Sign Language Acquisition[edit]

The ability to acquire speech is not the same as the ability to acquire language. The population’s primary means of communication is produced orally; however, speech and language are dissociative factors. [11] Although we are biologically equipped to use language, we are not biologically limited to speech.[12] A child who has no access to a spoken language will readily acquire a sign language, and a child deprived of both spoken and signed language sometimes invents his or her own gestural system of communication. [13]

There is an innate desire to produce language in both hearing and deaf population. All babies will use vocalizations to communicate.[14] Deaf children who have not been exposed to sign language create their own gesture communication known as homesign for the purpose of expressing what they are feeling. This term refers to gestures that are being used by deaf individuals who were reared in isolation from other deaf signers. Homesign is viewed as a biological component of language because it originates directly from the deaf child and because it is a global occurrence, transcending culture. [15]

American Sign language is a well known form of communication that is linguistic for both hearing and deaf individuals. [16] Deaf children learning ASL will go through a series of milestones in language from birth through one year of age. These milestones are similar to those of spoken language. A deaf child is aware of his/her environment, enjoys human interaction, smiles, and enjoys hand play from birth to 3 months of age. From 3-6 months a deaf child also begins to babble, referred to as finger babbling. [17] These gestures of the deaf children do not have real meaning, any more than babble noises have meaning, but they are more deliberate than the random finger flutters and fist clenches of hearing babies. (Angier, 1991) Between 6-12 months, deaf children will use manual communication, and will communicate with gestures, such as pulling and pointing. Many deaf children will sign their first word around 8 months of age and up to 10 or more signs by 12 months of age. [18]

Reading and Short-term Memory[edit]

Learning three-dimensional grammar, such as in ASL, boosts the child’s visual and spatial abilities to higher than average levels. [19] However, the frequently documented difficulty of learning to read may result from the requirement of pre-existing oral language for literacy.[20] In order to succeed in reading, the deaf child must have a strong language to base it upon. Additionally, communication difficulties with the teacher can impair reading.

Additionally, deaf children often show reduced short-term memory spans for written words in comparison to age-matched hearing children simple because they are less familiar with English words. [21] Short-term memory spans for signs and fingerspelling are also reduced in comparison to age-matched hearing children’s span for spoken words. Deaf children vary widely in their developmental experience with sign language, which affects development of short-term memory processes. Children who begin language acquisition at older ages and/or have limited language input during early childhood have underdeveloped sign language skill, which, in turn, affects their short-term memory development. [22]However, with the linguistic element removed, deaf children performance is equivalent to age-matched hearing children on short term memory tasks.

Children of Deaf Parents[edit]

Mother's who are deaf themselves model signs during face-to- face interactions with their deaf babies. They mold the hands of their babies to form shapes of signs. They exaggerate their facial expressions and provide their models in the direct line of vision of their deaf babies. [23] The caregivers of both hearing children and deaf children are believed to reinforce their children's early attempts at communication, thus encouraging further and more elaborate communication.

Deaf students who have deaf parents outperform their deaf peers who have hearing parents on every subtest of the WISC-R performance scale. [24] This is due to the fact that deaf parents are better prepared than hearing parents to meet the early learning needs of the deaf child; thus, they acquire language ‘on schedule’. Additionally, deaf children of deaf parents pass through language development stages earlier because the visual pathways are fully myelinated at an earlier age than the comparable auditory pathways.

Neuropsychological Function[edit]

Deaf children often have enhanced perceptual skills to compensate for the impaired auditory input, and this continues throughout adulthood. Congenitally deaf adults who used sign language showed ERPs that were 5-6 times larger than those of hearing adults over the Left and Right occipital regions. And ERPs 2-3 times larger than hearing participants over the left temporal and parietal regions (which are responsible for linguistic processing).[25] Because both hearing and deaf adults using ASL showed larger ERPs occipital regions, the heightened response to visual stimuli is also due to knowing and using sign language and not only due to deafness.

Both hearing and deaf adults using ASL also show larger ERPs over the left than right hemisphere. Since the left hemisphere is responsible for language, this implies that sign movement is linguistically salient. The movement processed on the left side (language) implies that the right visual field is stronger in deaf and hearing ASL due to the hemispheric association being contralateral. [26]


Sociocultural Factors[edit]

Deaf children from a lower SES are at a high risk for not being exposed to accessible language at the right time in early childhood. This is because in most countries poverty translates into a lack of access to the educational and clinical services that expose deaf children to language at the appropriate age.[27]

Academic achievement of deaf students is predicted to a large extent by the same factors that predict the academic achievement of normally hearing students, such as, social class, ethnic and racial backgrounds, and the presence of additional handicapping conditions. This means that deafness, by itself, does not determine academic success or failure but rather interacts with many other factors in complex ways. [28]

Early Intervention[edit]

The deaf children of hearing parents may not have significant exposure to any language in early childhood. Because of their sensory loss, these children perceive little of their parents’ speech. Because in most cases the parents do not sign the children are also not exposed to a conventional sign language. (Meier) Until recently, education of deaf emphasized speech training and the deaf children also were not exposed to sign language in school.

Not being exposed to accessible language at a certain time in early childhood combined with lack of access to the educational and clinical services that expose deaf children to language at the appropriate age are all factors that contribute to language acquisition of prelingually deaf individuals.[29]

  1. ^ Language development in exceptional circumstances (1. publ., reprinted in pbk. ed.). Hove [u.a.]: Erlbaum. 1994. ISBN 0-86377-308-7. {{cite book}}: |first= has generic name (help); |first= missing |last= (help)
  2. ^ Language development in exceptional circumstances (1. publ., reprinted in pbk. ed.). Hove [u.a.]: Erlbaum. 1994. ISBN 0-86377308-7. {{cite book}}: |first= has generic name (help); |first= missing |last= (help)
  3. ^ Mayberry, Rachel (2002). "4". In Segalowitz and Rapin (ed.). Handbook of Neuropsychology (2 ed.). chapter 4. pp. 71–107.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: location (link) CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link)
  4. ^ Margolis (2001). Implications of Prelingual Deafness. 358: Lancet. p. 76.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: location (link)
  5. ^ Mayberry, Rachel (2002). "4". In Segalowitz and Rapin (ed.). Handbook of Neuropsychology (2 ed.). chapter 4. pp. 71–107.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: location (link) CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link)
  6. ^ McKinley, Ann M.; Warren, Steven F. (1 January 2000). "The Effectiveness of Cochlear Implants for Children With Prelingual Deafness". Journal of Early Intervention. 23 (4): 252–263. doi:10.1177/10538151000230040501.
  7. ^ McKinley, Ann M.; Warren, Steven F. (1 January 2000). "The Effectiveness of Cochlear Implants for Children With Prelingual Deafness". Journal of Early Intervention. 23 (4): 252–263. doi:10.1177/10538151000230040501.
  8. ^ McKinley, Ann M.; Warren, Steven F. (1 January 2000). "The Effectiveness of Cochlear Implants for Children With Prelingual Deafness". Journal of Early Intervention. 23 (4): 252–263. doi:10.1177/10538151000230040501.
  9. ^ Mayberry, Rachel (2002). "4". In Segalowitz and Rapin (ed.). Handbook of Neuropsychology (2 ed.). chapter 4. pp. 71–107.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: location (link) CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link)
  10. ^ McKinley, Ann M.; Warren, Steven F. (1 January 2000). "The Effectiveness of Cochlear Implants for Children With Prelingual Deafness". Journal of Early Intervention. 23 (4): 252–263. doi:10.1177/10538151000230040501.
  11. ^ Rosenburg (2006). Baby Sign Language.
  12. ^ Meier, Richard (1991). "Language Acquisition by Deaf Children". American Scientist. 79 (1): 60–70. JSTOR 29774278.
  13. ^ Meier, Richard (1991). "Language Acquisition by Deaf Children". American Scientist. 79 (1): 60–70. JSTOR 29774278.
  14. ^ Mayberry, Rachel (2002). "4". In Segalowitz and Rapin (ed.). Handbook of Neuropsychology (2 ed.). chapter 4. pp. 71–107.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: location (link) CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link)
  15. ^ Mayberry, Rachel (2002). "4". In Segalowitz and Rapin (ed.). Handbook of Neuropsychology (2 ed.). chapter 4. pp. 71–107.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: location (link) CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link)
  16. ^ Mayberry, Rachel (2002). "4". In Segalowitz and Rapin (ed.). Handbook of Neuropsychology (2 ed.). chapter 4. pp. 71–107.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: location (link) CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link)
  17. ^ Andrews, Logan, and Phelan (2008). "Milestones of Language Development". Advance for Speech-Language Pathologists and Audiologists. 18 (2): 16–20.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  18. ^ Andrews, Logan, and Phelan (2008). "Milestones of Language Development". Advance for Speech-Language Pathologists and Audiologists. 18 (2): 16–20.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  19. ^ Mayberry, Rachel (2002). "4". In Segalowitz and Rapin (ed.). Handbook of Neuropsychology (2 ed.). chapter 4. pp. 71–107.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: location (link) CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link)
  20. ^ Language development in exceptional circumstances (1. publ., reprinted in pbk. ed.). Hove [u.a.]: Erlbaum. 1994. ISBN 0-86377-308-7. {{cite book}}: |first= has generic name (help); |first= missing |last= (help)
  21. ^ Mayberry, Rachel (2002). "4". In Segalowitz and Rapin (ed.). Handbook of Neuropsychology (2 ed.). chapter 4. pp. 71–107.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: location (link) CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link)
  22. ^ Meier, Richard (1991). "Language Acquisition by Deaf Children". American Scientist. 79 (1): 60–70. JSTOR 29774278.
  23. ^ Andrews, Logan, and Phelan (2008). "Milestones of Language Development". Advance for Speech-Language Pathologists and Audiologists. 18 (2): 16–20.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  24. ^ Mayberry, Rachel (2002). "4". In Segalowitz and Rapin (ed.). Handbook of Neuropsychology (2 ed.). chapter 4. pp. 71–107.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: location (link) CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link)
  25. ^ Neville, H. J. (NaN undefined NaN). "Cerebral organization for language in deaf and hearing subjects: Biological constraints and effects of experience". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 95 (3): 922–929. doi:10.1073/pnas.95.3.922. PMC 33817. PMID 9448260. {{cite journal}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)
  26. ^ Neville, H. J. (NaN undefined NaN). "Cerebral organization for language in deaf and hearing subjects: Biological constraints and effects of experience". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 95 (3): 922–929. doi:10.1073/pnas.95.3.922. PMC 33817. PMID 9448260. {{cite journal}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)
  27. ^ Mayberry, Rachel (2002). "4". In Segalowitz and Rapin (ed.). Handbook of Neuropsychology (2 ed.). chapter 4. pp. 71–107.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: location (link) CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link)
  28. ^ Mayberry, Rachel (2002). "4". In Segalowitz and Rapin (ed.). Handbook of Neuropsychology (2 ed.). chapter 4. pp. 71–107.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: location (link) CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link)
  29. ^ Mayberry, Rachel (2002). "4". In Segalowitz and Rapin (ed.). Handbook of Neuropsychology (2 ed.). chapter 4. pp. 71–107.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: location (link) CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link)